Counterfactual structure and learning from experience in negotiations
نویسندگان
چکیده
Reflecting on the past is often a critical ingredient for successful learning. The current research investigated how counterfactual thinking, reflecting on how prior experiences might have been different, motivates effective learning from these previous experiences. Specifically, we explored how the structure of counterfactual reflection – their additive (‘‘If only I had”) versus subtractive (‘‘If only I had not”) nature – influences performance in dyadic-level strategic interactions. Building on the functionalist account of counterfactuals, we found across two experiments that generating additive counterfactuals about a previous negotiation produced an advantage for negotiators over their previous performance compared to subtractive counterfactuals, both in terms of obtaining value for oneself and conceiving creative agreements. Additive counterfactuals enabled negotiators to more effectively extract lessons from past experiences to improve their current negotiation performance. ! 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Learning from experience often involves determining the causes of performance shortcomings to identify lessons to improve performance. A useful tool for establishing causal links between antecedents and outcomes is counterfactual thinking, or considerations of ‘‘what might have been” (Kahneman &Miller, 1986; Kray, Galinsky, & Wong, 2006; Markman & McMullen, 2003; Wells & Gavanski, 1989). Counterfactual thinking has proven to be a fundamental part of improving subsequent performance (Markman, Gavanski, Sherman, & McMullen, 1993). Whether it is students aiming to improve their scores on academic exams (Roese, 1994) or pilots learning from disasters barely averted (Morris & Moore, 2000), the generation of counterfactuals improves performance by helping to specify the necessary conditions to avoid repeating previous errors. One aspect of counterfactuals that determines their impact is the direction of the counterfactual, or whether a better versus worse possible world is imagined. Comparisons to worse possible worlds (downward counterfactuals) can help people feel better; whereas comparisons to better possible worlds (upward counterfactuals) can help to improve future performance (though at a temporary cost to affect). Although previous research has established that the generation of upward counterfactual thoughts about a past negotiation increases time devoted to preparing for subsequent negotiations (Galinsky, Seiden, Kim, & Medvec, 2002), surprisingly little attention has been paid to the effect of counterfactual thought on actual learning (i.e. performance gains) from past strategic interactions. The current research holds constant the direction of counterfactuals and examines how counterfactual structure (additive versus subtractive) impacts the learning process in strategic interactions. Whereas additive counterfactuals introduce new antecedent elements in constructing an alternate reality (e.g., ‘‘If only I had studied X topic, I would have gotten a better grade”), subtractive counterfactuals remove antecedent elements (e.g., ‘‘If only I had not studied this obscure topic, I would have gotten a better grade”) (Roese, Hur, & Pennington, 1999; Roese & Olson, 1993). In Roese (1994) influential research on the functions of counterfactuals, counterfactual direction and structure orthogonally impacted learning: both upward and additive counterfactuals improved performance on subsequent anagram tasks. We hypothesize that the generation of additive counterfactuals produces an advantage in mixed-motive interactions. By identifying alternative actions that would have resulted in success, additive counterfactuals provide a specific script for future action (Johnson & Sherman, 1990; Roese, 1994; Roese & Olson, 1993). In contrast, subtractive counterfactuals simply remove an option from consideration, thereby leaving negotiators with a more poorly specified roadmap for future action. Additive counterfactuals are also more creative than subtractive counterfactuals (Markman, Lindberg, Kray, & Galinsky, 2007; Roese, 1994). Whereas subtractive counterfactuals simply remove one response option from consideration, additive counterfactuals necessitate going beyond the original premise set to generate novel options not previously considered. Because negotiating effectively often involves a creative 0022-1031/$ see front matter ! 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.008 * Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (L.J. Kray). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 979–982
منابع مشابه
Knowing When to Ask: The Cost of Leaning-in
Gender differences in the propensity to negotiate are often used to explain the gender wage gap, popularizing the push for women to “lean-in.” We use a laboratory experiment to examine the effect of leaning-in. Despite men and women achieving similar and positive returns when they must negotiate, we find that women avoid negotiations more often than men. While this suggests that women would ben...
متن کاملLearning from other people's experience: A neuroimaging study of decisional interactive-learning
Decision-making is strongly influenced by the counterfactual anticipation of personal regret and relief, through a learning process involving the ventromedial-prefrontal cortex. We previously reported that observing the regretful outcomes of another's choices reactivates the regret-network. Here we extend those findings by investigating whether this resonant mechanism also underpins interactive...
متن کاملPitfalls of Counterfactual Thinking in Medical Practice: Preventing Errors by Using More Functional Reference Points
BACKGROUND Counterfactual thinking involves mentally simulating alternatives to reality. The current article reviews literature pertaining to the relevance counterfactual thinking has for the quality of medical decision making. Although earlier counterfactual thought research concluded that counterfactuals have important benefits for the individual, there are reasons to believe that counterfact...
متن کاملArticle Title: Measuring leadership styles in the organization with a approach of counterfactual thinking (Case study of Iranian Offshore Oil Company)
This research aims to determine leadership styles based on counterfactual thinking in relation to oneself and others. The research method was semi structure and the statistical population of the research were formed by the leaders of the Offshore Oil Company, among whom thirty-two people based on characteristics, managers The CEO was immediately elected. The research tools included eight leader...
متن کاملEmotion-Based Reinforcement Learning
Studies have shown that counterfactual reasoning can shape human decisions. However, there is a gap in the literature between counterfactual choices in description-based and experience-based paradigms. While studies using descriptionbased paradigms suggest participants maximize expected subjective emotion, studies using experience-based paradigms assume that participants learn the values of opt...
متن کامل